Volume 23, Issue 2 (Summer 2017)                   IJPCP 2017, 23(2): 148-163 | Back to browse issues page


XML Persian Abstract Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Amiri S, Isazadegan A, Yaghobi A, Abdollahi M H. The Effects of Cognitive Appraisal and Emotional Suppression on Autonomic Nervous Reactions on the Basis of Sensory Processing Sensitivity. IJPCP 2017; 23 (2) :148-163
URL: http://ijpcp.iums.ac.ir/article-1-2766-en.html
1- Department of Education, Faculty of Literature and Humanities, Urmia University , E-mail: ali_issazadeg@yahoo.com
2- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, Kharazmi University
Full-Text [PDF 3540 kb]   (3421 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (18651 Views)
Full-Text:   (8008 Views)
Extended Abstract
1. Introduction

Positive excitement regulation is associated with a number of health, social, and physiological outcomes [17, 18]. But, it is assumed that failure to regulate excitement is an underlying mechanism of mood and anxiety disorders [19]. Another important factor is individual differences in this connection. Sensory processing sensitivity is a personality mood feature, which is characterized by sensitivity to internal and external stimuli such as emotional and social stimuli that are the causes of its emergence. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of emotion regulation strategies, cognitive assessment and emotional suppression on auto nerve reactions based on high sensory processing sensitivity and low sensory processing sensitivity in the students.
2. Method
Given the type of variables and their manipulation, the current study belongs to empirical design method. Statistical population comprised all college students at the Bu-Ali Sina University in the academic year of 2015-16. Among them, 500 students were selected according to the Cohen table and in form of stratified sampling and with obtaining informed consent in order  to study the research objectives. Given the widespread of the population, ease of conducting research, and controlling gender variable, it is necessary to mention that only female gender was selected. After determining initial sample, sensory processing sensitivity scale was distributed among them. After analyzing the initial scores in the sensory processing sensitivity scale, 45 samples were randomly selected from the initial sample based on the distribution scores (Standard Z). 
After obtaining informed consent, the final sample was categorized into three groups of High Sensory Processing sensitivity (HSP), Low Sensitivity Processing sensitivity (LSP), and control group. Then, activation and inhibition behavior scale, list of positive and negative emotions, emotional expression and the cognitive order of excitement were given to the participants to respond. Then in the presence of each subject in the psychology lab that had psychometric properties and after removing disturbing triggers, the following steps were followed: 1) At the beginning of work, the participants were informed of the general process of research and were justified. In the next step, after eliminating the tension created in the participants, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and heart rate were measured three times in order to measure the base line; 2) The participants were then exposed to negative emotions and given the instruction of "normal observation"; 3) In the next stage, the participants were subjected to negative emotions and were given the instruction of "cognitive assessment"; 4) Then, the participants were exposed to the influence of negative excitement and were given the instruction of "emotional suppression" before showing the emotional movie piece. After running each of the steps 2, 3 and 4, heart rate and systolic and diastolic blood pressure were recorded three times. The obtained data was entered into the SPSS-22 computer software for analysis. In the analysis of information, multivariate covariance analysis, Ben Porney's pursuit test, and dependent t test were used in addition to the descriptive statistics indicators. According to this, baseline scores on systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and heart rate, activation and inhibition behavior scale, list of positive and negative emotions, emotional expressing and the cognitive order of excitement were used as auxiliary variables.
3. Results
In normal observation mode, the group with HSP has a higher heart rate than LSP group and also the control group. Also, in systolic blood pressure in suppressed mode, the group with HSP had a higher status, compared to the LSP group. The LSP group had a lower status in this component compared to the control group. In normal observation mode, suppression and cognitive assessment of negative excitement-inducing films, diastolic blood pressure was lower than the baseline mode with a significant difference in normal observation mode. It was also shown that suppression and cognitive assessment had lower diastolic blood pressure compared to normal observation mode. The comparison of heart rates in different modes of excitement regulation indicated a higher heart rate in baseline mode compared to the usual observation of the negative emotional film. In addition, the use of cognitive assessment and suppression increased heart rate compared to the normal observation mode; the increase was significant in cognitive assessment.
4. Discussion
Given that patients with HSP have higher sensitivity towards stimuli provided, it seems that they are the most affected by negative emotions resulting in a higher physiological response in auto nerve reactions. Studies have shown that inducing stress as a negative excitement significantly increased systolic and diastolic blood pressure of subjects. Stressors send signals to Central Nervous System (CNS) and create a quick response through the auto nerve system. Therefore, sympatric-adrenal-modulus axis affects organs through direct and quick stimulation of sympatric nerve (such as widening of blood vessels, increasing artery blood pressure). 
Using cognitive assessment and suppression, compared to normal observation mode, causes an increase in the heart rate. The present study showed that suppression and cognitive assessment, compared to normal observation mode, reduced diastolic blood pressure. This finding is in line with studies that have shown that cognitive regulation neutralized negative emotional experiences and reduced physiological arousal [17]. Relevance of this characteristic is close to heartbeat, and the influence of these two indicators is from the activity of a similar autonomous machine. Similar to our findings, Augustine et al. [64] also showed that people who experienced more negative emotions in life got infected sooner if they were prone to illness. Overall, the findings of this study showed that automatic nerve response can be affected by the type of emotional regulation strategy and biological traits. To improve the understanding of functional organization of ANS activity in excitement, future research should be taken into consideration, and if possible, the type of excitement and also the individual differences should be examined. 
The limitations of the present study were that sampling was carried out voluntarily and in form of convenient sampling. This study has been carried out only on female subjects, and from this point of view, care should be taken while generalizing the results to other demographic groups. Selecting people for the final stage was based on self-reporting tool, though this tool has been confirmed to be valid to measure the desired variable. It is suggested that other tools like interviewing, reporting by others and physiological evaluation need to be used in future studies. Through the examination of other psychological and physiological variables, the pattern of relationship between personality, excitement regulation and the automated nervous system should be expanded.
Acknowledgments
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declared no conflicts of interest.

 
References
  1. Kring AM, Sloan DM. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. 
  2. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000; 61(3):201–16. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4
  3. Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007.
  4. Kreibig SD. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology. 2010; 84(3):394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010
  5. Hagemann D, Waldstein SR, Thayer JF. Central and autonomic nervous system integration in emotion. Brain and Cognition. 2003; 52(1):79–87. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00011-3
  6. Thayer JF, Yamamoto SS, Brosschot JF. The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. International Journal of Cardiology. 2010; 141(2):122–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543
  7. Brosschot JF, Gerin W, Thayer JF. The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006; 60(2):113–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074
  8. Kok BE, Fredrickson BL. Upward spirals of the heart: Autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biological Psychology. 2010; 85(3):432–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005
  9. McCraty R, Rees RA. The central role of the heart in generating and sustaining positive emotions. In: Snyder CR, Lopez S, editors. Oxford handbook of positive psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0050
  10. Deschênes SS, Dugas MJ, Gouin J-P. Intolerance of uncertainty, worry catastrophizing, and heart rate variability during worry-inducing tasks. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016; 90:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.015
  11. Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart–brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009; 33(2):81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004
  12. Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology. 2006; 10(3):229–40. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229
  13. Cristea IA, Valenza G, Scilingo EP, Szentágotai Tătar A, Gentili C, David D. Autonomic effects of cognitive reappraisal and acceptance in social anxiety: Evidence for common and distinct pathways for parasympathetic reactivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2014; 28(8):795–803. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.09.009
  14. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2008; 59(2-3):25–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x
  15. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment . 2008; 30(4):315–315. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9102-4
  16. Gross JJ. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998; 74(1):224–37. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
  17. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. 2002; 39(3):281–91. doi: 10.1017/s0048577201393198
  18. Gross JJ. Handbook of emotion regulation. NewYork: Guilford Press; 2006.
  19. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH. Incorporating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In: Gross JJ editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007.
  20. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003; 85(2):348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
  21. Melnick SM, Hinshaw SP. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2000; 28(1):73–86. doi: 10.1023/a:1005174102794
  22. Fraire MG, Ollendick TH. Anxiety and oppositional defiant disorder: A transdiagnostic conceptualization. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013; 33(2):229–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.004
  23. Gruber J, Eidelman P, Harvey AG. Transdiagnostic emotion regulation processes in bipolar disorder and insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008; 46(9):1096–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.004
  24. Svaldi J, Griepenstroh J, Tuschen-Caffier B, Ehring T. Emotion regulation deficits in eating disorders: A marker of eating pathology or general psychopathology. Psychiatry Research. 2012; 197(1-2):103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.009
  25. Fairholme CP, Nosen EL, Nillni YI, Schumacher JA, Tull MT, Coffey SF. Sleep disturbance and emotion dysregulation as transdiagnostic processes in a comorbid sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2013; 51(9):540–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.014
  26. Jagiellowicz J, Xu X, Aron A, Aron E, Cao G, Feng T, et al. The trait of sensory processing sensitivity and neural responses to changes in visual scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2010; 6(1):38–47. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq001
  27. Aron EN, Aron A. Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997; 73(2):345–68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.345
  28. Dunn W. The sensations of everyday life: Empirical, theoretical, and pragmatic considerations. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2001; 55(6):608–20. doi: 10.5014/ajot.55.6.608
  29. Sih A, Bell AM. Chapter 5 insights for behavioral ecology from behavioral syndromes. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2008; 227–81. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(08)00005-3
  30. Wolf M, van Doorn GS, Weissing FJ. Evolutionary emergence of responsive and unresponsive personalities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008; 105(41):15825–30. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805473105
  31. Smolewska KA, McCabe SB, Woody EZ. A psychometric evaluation of the highly sensitive person scale: The components of sensory-processing sensitivity and their relation to the BIS/BAS and “Big Five”. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006; 40(6):1269–79. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.022
  32. Bakker K, Moulding R. Sensory-processing sensitivity, dispositional mindfulness and negative psychological symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences. 2012; 53(3):341–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.006
  33. Benham G. The highly sensitive person: Stress and physical symptom reports. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006; 40(7):1433–40. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.021
  34. Hofmann SG, Bitran S. Sensory-processing sensitivity in social anxiety disorder: Relationship to harm avoidance and diagnostic subtypes. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2007; 21(7):944–54. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.003
  35. Meyer B, Ajchenbrenner M, Bowles DP. Sensory sensitivity, attachment experiences, and rejection responses among adults with borderline and avoidant features. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2005; 19(6):641–58. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2005.19.6.641
  36. Neal JA, Edelmann RJ, Glachan M. Behavioural inhibition and symptoms of anxiety and depression: Is there a specific relationship with social phobia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2002; 41(4):361–74. doi: 10.1348/014466502760387489
  37. Watson D, Clark LA, Harkness AR. Structures of personality and their relevance to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994; 103(1):18–31. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.103.1.18
  38. Stemmler G. Methodological considerations in the psychophysiological study of emotion. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith H, editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
  39. Gianaros PJ, Quigley KS. Autonomic origins of a nonsignal stimulus-elicited bradycardia and its habituation in humans. Psychophysiology. 2001; 38(3):540–7. doi: 10.1017/s004857720100004x
  40. Pine DS, Wasserman GA, Miller L, Coplan JD, Bagiella E, Kovelenku P, et al. Heart period variability and psychopathology in urban boys at risk for delinquency. Psychophysiology. 1998; 35(5):521–9. doi: 10.1017/s0048577298970846
  41. Fabes RA, Eisenberg N, Eisenbud L. Behavioral and physiological correlates of children's reactions to others in distress. Developmental Psychology. 1993; 29(4):655-63. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.29.4.655
  42. Dolan RJ. Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science. 2002; 298(5596):1191–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1076358
  43. Schmeichel BJ, Volokhov RN, Demaree HA. Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008; 95(6):1526–40. doi: 10.1037/a0013345
  44. Mauss IB, Levenson RW, McCarter L, Wilhelm FH, Gross JJ. The tie that binds: Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion. 2005; 5(2):175–90. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175
  45. Hasani J. [The impact of reappraisal and suppression of emotional experiences on brain activity by looking at the dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism area (Persian)] [PhD thesis]. Tehran: Tarbiat Modares University; 2008.
  46. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994; 67(2):319–33. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319
  47. Johnson JL, Kim LM, Giovannelli TS, Cagle T. Reinforcement sensitivity theory, vengeance, and forgiveness. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010; 48(5):612–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.018
  48. Amiri S, Hassani J. [Assessment of psychometric properties of behavioral activation and behavioral inhibition systems scale associated with impulsivity and anxiety (Persian)]. Razi Journal of Medical Sciences. 2016; 23(144):68-80.
  49. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988; 54(6):1063–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
  50. Bakhshipoor A, Dezhkam M. [A confirmatory factor analysis of the positive affect and negative affect scales (Persian)]. Journal of Psychology; 2005, 9(4):351-65. 
  51. Besharat, MA. [Assessment of psychometric properties of the Persian version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; Research report (Persian)]. Tehran: University of Tehran; 2008.
  52. King LA, Emmons RA. Conflict over emotional expression: Psychological and physical correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990; 58(5):864–77. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.864
  53. Rafienia P. [The relationship between emotional expression styles with general health of students (Persian)] [MA thesis]. Tehran: Tarbiat Modarres University, 2002.
  54. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001; 30(8):1311–27. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00113-6
  55. Hasani J. [The reliability and validity of the short form of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (Persian)]. Journal of Research in Behavioural Sciences. 2011; 9(4):229-40.
  56. Liss M, Mailloux J, Erchull MJ. The relationships between sensory processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008; 45(3):255–9. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.009
  57. Guyton AC, Hall JE. Textbook of medical physiology. Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders; 2006.
  58. Donker FJ. Cardiac rehabilitation. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000; 20(7):923–43. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00016-1
  59. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, McGuire L, Robles TF, Glaser R. Emotions, morbidity, and mortality: New perspectives from psychoneuroimmunology. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002; 53(1):83–107. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135217
  60. Carnethon MR. Symptoms of depression as a risk factor for incident diabetes: Findings from the national health and nutrition examination epidemiologic follow-up study, 1971-1992. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003; 158(5):416–23. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg172
  61. Chesney MA, Darbes LA, Hoerster K, Taylor JM, Chambers DB, Anderson DE. Positive emotions: Exploring the other hemisphere in behavioral medicine. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2005; 12(2):50–8. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_2
  62. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health.Psychological Bulletin. 2005; 131(6):925–71. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925
  63. Cacioppo JT, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Uchino BN, Sgoutas-Emch SA, Sheridan JF, et al. Heterogeneity in neuroendocrine and immune responses to brief psychological stressors as a function of autonomic cardiac activation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1995; 57(2):154–64. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199503000-00008
  64. Augustine AA, Larsen RJ, Walker MS, Fisher EB. Personality predictors of the time course for lung cancer onset. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008; 42(6):1448–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.006
  65. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997; 106(1):95–103. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.106.1.95
  66. Khalfa S, Roy M, Rainville P, Dalla Bella S, Peretz I. Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of happy and sad music. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2008; 68(1):17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001
  67. Xiu L, Zhou R, Jiang Y. Working memory training improves emotion regulation ability: Evidence from HRV. Physiology & Behavior. 2016; 155:25–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.004
  68. Di Simplicio M, Costoloni G, Western D, Hanson B, Taggart P, Harmer CJ. Decreased heart rate variability during emotion regulation in subjects at risk for psychopathology. Psychological Medicine. 2011; 42(8):1775–83. doi: 10.1017/s0033291711002479
  69. Lane R, Mcrae K, Reiman E, Chen K, Ahern G, Thayer J. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. NeuroImage. 2009; 44(1):213–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056
  70. Thayer JF, Åhs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012; 36(2):747–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009
  71. Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008; 59(6):1037–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006
  72. Chalmers JA, Quintana DS, Abbott MJ-A, Kemp AH. Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2014; 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00080
  73. Segerstrom SC, Smith TW, Eisenlohr-Moul TA. Positive psychophysiology. In: Sheldon KM, Kashdan TB, Steger MF, editors. Designing Positive Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.003.0003
  74. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH, Brown TA, Hofmann SG. Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006; 44(9):1251–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001
  75. Friedman BH, Thayer JF. Anxiety and autonomic flexibility: A cardiovascular approach. Biological Psychology. 1998; 47(3):243–63. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(97)00027-6
  76. Thayer JF, Friedman BH, Borkovec TD. Autonomic characteristics of generalized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological Psychiatry. 1996; 39(4):255–66. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00136-0
  77. Rottenberg J. Cardiac vagal control in depression: A critical analysis. Biological Psychology. 2007; 74(2):200–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010
  78. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: Is the bark worse than the bite. Journal of Personality. 1987; 55(2):299–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00438.x 
  79. Berntson GG, Thomas Bigger J, Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, et al. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 1997; 34(6):623–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x
  80. James GD, Yee LS, Harshfield GA, Blank SG, Pickering TG. The influence of happiness, anger, and anxiety on the blood pressure of borderline hypertensives. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1986; 48(7):502–8. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198609000-00005
  81. Barrett LF. Are emotions natural kinds. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2006; 1(1):28–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x
Type of Study: Original Research | Subject: Psychiatry and Psychology
Received: 2016/06/28 | Accepted: 2016/12/7 | Published: 2017/07/1

References
1. Kring AM, Sloan DM. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2009.
2. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000; 61(3):201–16. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4 [DOI:10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4]
3. Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007.
4. Kreibig SD. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology. 2010; 84(3):394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010 [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010]
5. Hagemann D, Waldstein SR, Thayer JF. Central and autonomic nervous system integration in emotion. Brain and Cognition. 2003; 52(1):79–87. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00011-3 [DOI:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00011-3]
6. Thayer JF, Yamamoto SS, Brosschot JF. The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. International Journal of Cardiology. 2010; 141(2):122–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543 [DOI:10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543]
7. Brosschot JF, Gerin W, Thayer JF. The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006; 60(2):113–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074 [DOI:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074]
8. Kok BE, Fredrickson BL. Upward spirals of the heart: Autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biological Psychology. 2010; 85(3):432–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005 [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005]
9. McCraty R, Rees RA. The central role of the heart in generating and sustaining positive emotions. In: Snyder CR, Lopez S, editors. Oxford handbook of positive psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0050 [DOI:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0050]
10. Deschênes SS, Dugas MJ, Gouin J-P. Intolerance of uncertainty, worry catastrophizing, and heart rate variability during worry-inducing tasks. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016; 90:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.015 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.015]
11. Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart–brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009; 33(2):81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004 [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004]
12. Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology. 2006; 10(3):229–40. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229 [DOI:10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229]
13. Cristea IA, Valenza G, Scilingo EP, Szentágotai Tătar A, Gentili C, David D. Autonomic effects of cognitive reappraisal and acceptance in social anxiety: Evidence for common and distinct pathways for parasympathetic reactivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2014; 28(8):795–803. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.09.009 [DOI:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.09.009]
14. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2008; 59(2-3):25–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x [DOI:10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x]
15. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment . 2008; 30(4):315–315. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9102-4 [DOI:10.1007/s10862-008-9102-4]
16. Gross JJ. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998; 74(1):224–37. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224 [DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224]
17. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. 2002; 39(3):281–91. doi: 10.1017/s0048577201393198 [DOI:10.1017/S0048577201393198]
18. Gross JJ. Handbook of emotion regulation. NewYork: Guilford Press; 2006.
19. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH. Incorporating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In: Gross JJ editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. [PMID]
20. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003; 85(2):348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348]
21. Melnick SM, Hinshaw SP. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2000; 28(1):73–86. doi: 10.1023/a:1005174102794 [DOI:10.1023/A:1005174102794]
22. Fraire MG, Ollendick TH. Anxiety and oppositional defiant disorder: A transdiagnostic conceptualization. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013; 33(2):229–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.004 [DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.004]
23. Gruber J, Eidelman P, Harvey AG. Transdiagnostic emotion regulation processes in bipolar disorder and insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008; 46(9):1096–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.004 [DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.004]
24. Svaldi J, Griepenstroh J, Tuschen-Caffier B, Ehring T. Emotion regulation deficits in eating disorders: A marker of eating pathology or general psychopathology. Psychiatry Research. 2012; 197(1-2):103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.009 [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.009]
25. Fairholme CP, Nosen EL, Nillni YI, Schumacher JA, Tull MT, Coffey SF. Sleep disturbance and emotion dysregulation as transdiagnostic processes in a comorbid sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2013; 51(9):540–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.014 [DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.014]
26. Jagiellowicz J, Xu X, Aron A, Aron E, Cao G, Feng T, et al. The trait of sensory processing sensitivity and neural responses to changes in visual scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2010; 6(1):38–47. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq001 [DOI:10.1093/scan/nsq001]
27. Aron EN, Aron A. Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997; 73(2):345–68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.345 [DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.345]
28. Dunn W. The sensations of everyday life: Empirical, theoretical, and pragmatic considerations. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2001; 55(6):608–20. doi: 10.5014/ajot.55.6.608 [DOI:10.5014/ajot.55.6.608]
29. Sih A, Bell AM. Chapter 5 insights for behavioral ecology from behavioral syndromes. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2008; 227–81. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(08)00005-3 [DOI:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00005-3]
30. Wolf M, van Doorn GS, Weissing FJ. Evolutionary emergence of responsive and unresponsive personalities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008; 105(41):15825–30. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805473105 [DOI:10.1073/pnas.0805473105]
31. Smolewska KA, McCabe SB, Woody EZ. A psychometric evaluation of the highly sensitive person scale: The components of sensory-processing sensitivity and their relation to the BIS/BAS and "Big Five". Personality and Individual Differences. 2006; 40(6):1269–79. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.022 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.022]
32. Bakker K, Moulding R. Sensory-processing sensitivity, dispositional mindfulness and negative psychological symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences. 2012; 53(3):341–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.006 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.006]
33. Benham G. The highly sensitive person: Stress and physical symptom reports. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006; 40(7):1433–40. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.021 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.021]
34. Hofmann SG, Bitran S. Sensory-processing sensitivity in social anxiety disorder: Relationship to harm avoidance and diagnostic subtypes. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2007; 21(7):944–54. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.003 [DOI:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.003]
35. Meyer B, Ajchenbrenner M, Bowles DP. Sensory sensitivity, attachment experiences, and rejection responses among adults with borderline and avoidant features. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2005; 19(6):641–58. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2005.19.6.641 [DOI:10.1521/pedi.2005.19.6.641]
36. Neal JA, Edelmann RJ, Glachan M. Behavioural inhibition and symptoms of anxiety and depression: Is there a specific relationship with social phobia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2002; 41(4):361–74. doi: 10.1348/014466502760387489 [DOI:10.1348/014466502760387489]
37. Watson D, Clark LA, Harkness AR. Structures of personality and their relevance to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994; 103(1):18–31. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.103.1.18 [DOI:10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.18]
38. Stemmler G. Methodological considerations in the psychophysiological study of emotion. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith H, editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
39. Gianaros PJ, Quigley KS. Autonomic origins of a nonsignal stimulus-elicited bradycardia and its habituation in humans. Psychophysiology. 2001; 38(3):540–7. doi: 10.1017/s004857720100004x [DOI:10.1017/S004857720100004X]
40. Pine DS, Wasserman GA, Miller L, Coplan JD, Bagiella E, Kovelenku P, et al. Heart period variability and psychopathology in urban boys at risk for delinquency. Psychophysiology. 1998; 35(5):521–9. doi: 10.1017/s0048577298970846 [DOI:10.1017/S0048577298970846]
41. Fabes RA, Eisenberg N, Eisenbud L. Behavioral and physiological correlates of children's reactions to others in distress. Developmental Psychology. 1993; 29(4):655-63. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.29.4.655 [DOI:10.1037//0012-1649.29.4.655]
42. Dolan RJ. Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science. 2002; 298(5596):1191–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1076358 [DOI:10.1126/science.1076358]
43. Schmeichel BJ, Volokhov RN, Demaree HA. Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008; 95(6):1526–40. doi: 10.1037/a0013345 [DOI:10.1037/a0013345]
44. Mauss IB, Levenson RW, McCarter L, Wilhelm FH, Gross JJ. The tie that binds: Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion. 2005; 5(2):175–90. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175 [DOI:10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175]
45. Hasani J. [The impact of reappraisal and suppression of emotional experiences on brain activity by looking at the dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism area (Persian)] [PhD thesis]. Tehran: Tarbiat Modares University; 2008.
46. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994; 67(2):319–33. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 [DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319]
47. Johnson JL, Kim LM, Giovannelli TS, Cagle T. Reinforcement sensitivity theory, vengeance, and forgiveness. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010; 48(5):612–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.018 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.018]
48. Amiri S, Hassani J. [Assessment of psychometric properties of behavioral activation and behavioral inhibition systems scale associated with impulsivity and anxiety (Persian)]. Razi Journal of Medical Sciences. 2016; 23(144):68-80.
49. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988; 54(6):1063–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063]
50. Bakhshipoor A, Dezhkam M. [A confirmatory factor analysis of the positive affect and negative affect scales (Persian)]. Journal of Psychology; 2005, 9(4):351-65. 51. Besharat, MA. [Assessment of psychometric properties of the Persian version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; Research report (Persian)]. Tehran: University of Tehran; 2008.
51. King LA, Emmons RA. Conflict over emotional expression: Psychological and physical correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990; 58(5):864–77. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.864 [DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.864]
52. Rafienia P. [The relationship between emotional expression styles with general health of students (Persian)] [MA thesis]. Tehran: Tarbiat Modarres University, 2002.
53. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001; 30(8):1311–27. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00113-6 [DOI:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6]
54. Hasani J. [The reliability and validity of the short form of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (Persian)]. Journal of Research in Behavioural Sciences. 2011; 9(4):229-40.
55. Liss M, Mailloux J, Erchull MJ. The relationships between sensory processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008; 45(3):255–9. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.009 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.009]
56. Guyton AC, Hall JE. Textbook of medical physiology. Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders; 2006.
57. Donker FJ. Cardiac rehabilitation. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000; 20(7):923–43. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00016-1 [DOI:10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00016-1]
58. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, McGuire L, Robles TF, Glaser R. Emotions, morbidity, and mortality: New perspectives from psychoneuroimmunology. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002; 53(1):83–107. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135217 [DOI:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135217]
59. Carnethon MR. Symptoms of depression as a risk factor for incident diabetes: Findings from the national health and nutrition examination epidemiologic follow-up study, 1971-1992. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003; 158(5):416–23. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg172 [DOI:10.1093/aje/kwg172]
60. Chesney MA, Darbes LA, Hoerster K, Taylor JM, Chambers DB, Anderson DE. Positive emotions: Exploring the other hemisphere in behavioral medicine. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2005; 12(2):50–8. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_2 [DOI:10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_2]
61. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health.Psychological Bulletin. 2005; 131(6):925–71. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925 [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925]
62. Cacioppo JT, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Uchino BN, Sgoutas-Emch SA, Sheridan JF, et al. Heterogeneity in neuroendocrine and immune responses to brief psychological stressors as a function of autonomic cardiac activation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1995; 57(2):154–64. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199503000-00008 [DOI:10.1097/00006842-199503000-00008]
63. Augustine AA, Larsen RJ, Walker MS, Fisher EB. Personality predictors of the time course for lung cancer onset. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008; 42(6):1448–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.006 [DOI:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.006]
64. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997; 106(1):95–103. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.106.1.95 [DOI:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95]
65. Khalfa S, Roy M, Rainville P, Dalla Bella S, Peretz I. Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of happy and sad music. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2008; 68(1):17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001 [DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001]
66. Xiu L, Zhou R, Jiang Y. Working memory training improves emotion regulation ability: Evidence from HRV. Physiology & Behavior. 2016; 155:25–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.004 [DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.004]
67. Di Simplicio M, Costoloni G, Western D, Hanson B, Taggart P, Harmer CJ. Decreased heart rate variability during emotion regulation in subjects at risk for psychopathology. Psychological Medicine. 2011; 42(8):1775–83. doi: 10.1017/s0033291711002479 [DOI:10.1017/S0033291711002479]
68. Lane R, Mcrae K, Reiman E, Chen K, Ahern G, Thayer J. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. NeuroImage. 2009; 44(1):213–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056 [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056]
69. Thayer JF, Åhs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012; 36(2):747–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009 [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009]
70. Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008; 59(6):1037–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006 [DOI:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006]
71. Chalmers JA, Quintana DS, Abbott MJ-A, Kemp AH. Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2014; 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00080 [DOI:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00080]
72. Segerstrom SC, Smith TW, Eisenlohr-Moul TA. Positive psychophysiology. In: Sheldon KM, Kashdan TB, Steger MF, editors. Designing Positive Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.003.0003 [DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.003.0003]
73. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH, Brown TA, Hofmann SG. Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006; 44(9):1251–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001 [DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001]
74. Friedman BH, Thayer JF. Anxiety and autonomic flexibility: A cardiovascular approach. Biological Psychology. 1998; 47(3):243–63. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(97)00027-6 [DOI:10.1016/S0301-0511(97)00027-6]
75. Thayer JF, Friedman BH, Borkovec TD. Autonomic characteristics of generalized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological Psychiatry. 1996; 39(4):255–66. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00136-0 [DOI:10.1016/0006-3223(95)00136-0]
76. Rottenberg J. Cardiac vagal control in depression: A critical analysis. Biological Psychology. 2007; 74(2):200–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010 [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010]
77. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: Is the bark worse than the bite. Journal of Personality. 1987; 55(2):299–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00438.x [DOI:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00438.x]
78. Berntson GG, Thomas Bigger J, Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, et al. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 1997; 34(6):623–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x]
79. James GD, Yee LS, Harshfield GA, Blank SG, Pickering TG. The influence of happiness, anger, and anxiety on the blood pressure of borderline hypertensives. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1986; 48(7):502–8. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198609000-00005 [DOI:10.1097/00006842-198609000-00005]
80. Barrett LF. Are emotions natural kinds. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2006; 1(1):28–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x [DOI:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x]

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2024 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb